Or search by topic
Published 2018 Revised 2022
Young children have a remarkable skill: they can recognise numbers of things without counting. This is called subitising, and it develops from a very early age. Very young babies can not only tell the difference between one and two but also between large numbers of dots when there are twice as many in one group, as with 16 and 8 (Sarama and Clements, 2009). Young children also have powerful
visual memories and some may find it easier to remember images than words: three-year-olds can recognise three things, although they may not say the word. Subitising can help children to build images for numbers, to visualise and to learn number facts. For instance, most four-year-olds readily learn to recognise five dots on a dice, which helps them to understand the cardinal value or
'howmanyness' of five, which they can link to the word and symbol for 5. Structured images like this also help children to begin to see numbers inside numbers, for instance seeing four and one within five.
How do you teach subitising?
Children's ability to subitise can be developed by teaching. Games provide repeated experience of interpreting the dot images, either to decide how many spaces to move, as in our activity Playing Incey Wincey Spider, or how many things to collect, as in our activity Dice, where the dotty number determines how many animals to put in your pen. (To find both of these activities, scroll down the
collection of number based EYFS tasks.) For children who are unfamiliar with dot images, dice can be adapted to just show numbers up to three. Large dotty dice are good for outdoor games, like jumping along the track. Spinners with dots on provide an alternative, as with the child in the picture, who has his own spinner on a lanyard: he spins it and then makes that
many jumps around the circuit track, then does the same again and again and again..! Having your own dice or spinner is a good option for younger children and avoids waiting for turns.
Large floor dominoes invite children to match number patterns, indoors or out.
Fingers also provide subitisable images for numbers, with the added advantage that they are embodied in muscle memory. Showing all-at-once finger numbers is the key skill here: Marton & Neuman (1990) found that older children with maths difficulties tended to count fingers one at a time, rather than using all-at-once 'finger numbers'. You can ask young children to first 'grow', then 'show'
and finally to 'throw' finger numbers.
Using a range of objects and apparatus in different arrangements helps to make children familiar with different images for numbers, while also emphasising different number properties. For instance, Numicon presents numbers as even or odd, whereas linked cubes show numbers as lengths: connecting all these images helps children build a rich network of understanding to link to number words and
symbols.
Subitising games can involve less common arrangements of numbers of objects. Games which involve revealing hidden objects will prompt young children to say how many they see (see the NRICH activity Hidden Jewels). One game involves upturned dishes hiding three, four and five objects, rapidly swopping them around and challenging children to keep track of the dish with four.
When you stop and briefly lift the dish, children intuitively subitise the number revealed. This activity is taken from Clements and Sarama's very useful website, which presents a learning trajectory for subitising, supported by videos of activities for each stage. (If you have not already done so, you will need to create an account before accessing the free resources.)
https://www.learningtrajectories.org/math-activities/fantastic-five-tricky-two
Activities include auditory subitising, or recognising numbers of sounds, such as claps or drum beats, linking with movement and music.
Conceptual subitising
Subitising can also help children learn addition and subtraction facts. In some of the images below the total number of five is recognised by adding subgroups, like three and two, or four and one (Back, 2014 - see the NRICH article Early Number Sense). This involves part-whole awareness, which enables children to simultaneously see the whole number and numbers which make it
up. The ability to think about numbers as compositions of other numbers was described by Resnick (1983) as 'probably the major conceptual achievement of the early school years'.
Recognising how many there are by combining numbers of smaller subgroups is called conceptual subitising: this is more advanced than perceptual subitising, which involves instant recognition (Clements, 1999). Seeing numbers as combinations of other numbers helps children to develop part-whole awareness and build familiarity with number combinations. It is also much more effective than giving
children number sentences to complete by counting cubes and hoping they will remember the numbers involved!
Number talks
Showing children 'quick images' and asking them how many they see is one way of developing subitising. An alternative way of developing conceptual subitising is to show children a number of dots and ask them to say how they see the number (see the NRICH activity Number Talks). Teacher Janine Davenall engages young children by showing an image and asking, 'What numbers can you
see hidden inside this number?'
They may reply, 'I see three and four', or 'six and one' or 'two and two and two and one'. This is an inclusive activity with many 'right' answers and the opportunity to discover that others see the same thing differently. Children also learn about all the different combinations which can make the same number. An alternative is to use giant magnetic buttons on a tray, which you turn over to
secretly rearrange and show different combinations of the same number.
(www.teachingchannel.org/videos/visualizing-number-combinations)
Not only do younger children respond more to real objects than to images on a screen, but they can also see that the number of buttons stays the same despite being rearranged.
More complex images can present greater challenges for some children. Mulligan and Mitchelmore (2009) showed young children a 6 dot triangle and asked to draw it from memory: they found their responses varied significantly, with some just drawing a lot of dots.
Even when asked to copy the image, many five-year-olds produce dotty triangles like the one on the right, apparently finding it hard to focus on the number and the arrangement of the dots (Gifford & Thouless, 2018). This implies that some children will need a lot more practice in looking and talking about number patterns, in order to see how they are organised. Daily 'number talks' can help
children to analyse and visualise number images, building an understanding of cardinal values, like the 'sixness' of six.
Reception children enjoy creating their own arrangements for numbers like seven and nine, and seeing which their friends recognise most easily. Older children with mathematics difficulties found the 'lollipop seven' image most memorable, at the same time learning a key number fact, that four and three make seven (Emerson & Babtie, 2010).
In Hungary, children are taught to recognise such numbers as 'five and a bit', using the dice image for 5 and linking with finger numbers (see the NRICH article From Objects and Images to Mathematical Ideas).
In the Netherlands, researchers developed bead-strings and rekenreks (counting frames) using groups of five, as they found these helped children to 'see' numbers more quickly (Gravemeijer, 1991). Being able to subitise groups supports children's understanding of larger numbers and later multiplicative thinking: for instance, recognising larger numbers by counting groups of two, three or
five.
(The above images are taken from the Number Talk Images website.)
Subitising with everyday objects is more challenging than with simpler shapes, like dots. However, it can be more engaging for children to discuss images which are of particular interest and relevance to them, or even to create their own images for number talks.
Acknowledgments:
Janine Davenall
Buckinghamshire, Berkshire & Oxfordshire Mathematical Pedagogy Work Group
References:
Back, J. (2014) Early Number Sense
Clements, D.H. (1999) Subitising: What is it? Why teach it? Teaching Children Mathematics 5(7) 400-405
Emerson, J. & Babtie, P. (2010) The dyscalculia assessment: Emerson House Mathematics London: Continuum.
Thouless, H. & Gifford, S. (2018) 'Dotty triangles: two different approaches to analysing young children's responses to a pattern replication activity' in Golding, J., Bretscher, N., Crisan, C., Geraniou, E., Hodgen J. and C. Morgan (Eds). (2018) Research Proceedings
of the 9th British Congress on Mathematics Education (3-6 April 2018, University of Warwick, UK) 175-182 Online at www.bsrlm.org.uk/bcme-9/
Gravemeijer, K.P.E. (1991) An instruction-theoretical reflection on the use of manipulatives in Streefland, L. Realistic mathematics education in primary school. Utrecht: Freudenthal Institute 57- 76
Marton, F. & Neuman, D. (1990) 'Constructivism, phenomenology and the origin of arithmetic skills' in Steffe, L.P. & Wood, T. (1990) Transforming children's mathematics education: international perspectives NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates 62- 75
Resnick, L.B. (1983) 'A developmental theory of number understanding' in Ginsburg, H. (ed) The development of mathematical thinking. London Academic Press 109 - 181.
Sarama, J. S., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood mathematics education research. London: Routledge.
Download pdf here.